Apa Review Confirms Link Between Playing Violent Video Games and Aggression

  • Periodical List
  • Wiley-Blackwell Online Open
  • PMC6790614

Aggress Behav. 2019 Nov-Dec; 45(6): 635–642.

The contagious impact of playing violent video games on aggression: Longitudinal prove

Tobias Greitemeyer

1 Department of Psychology, Academy of Innsbruck, Innsbruck Austria,

Received 2019 Mar xiv; Revised 2019 Jun 7; Accepted 2019 Jun 12.

Abstract

Meta‐analyses have shown that violent video game play increases aggression in the player. The present research suggests that violent video game play as well affects individuals with whom the player is continued. A longitudinal study (N = 980) asked participants to report on their corporeality of violent video game play and level of aggression every bit well equally how they perceive their friends and examined the association between the participant'south aggression and their friends' amount of violent video game play. As hypothesized, friends' amount of violent video game play at Time one was associated with the participant's assailment at Time 2 fifty-fifty when controlling for the impact of the participant'due south aggression at Fourth dimension 1. Mediation analyses showed that friends' assailment at Time 1 accounted for the impact of friends' amount of trigger-happy video game play at Time i on the participant'due south assailment at Fourth dimension 2. These findings advise that increased aggression in video game players has an bear on on the player's social network.

Keywords: aggression, contamination, longitudinal data, violent video games

1. INTRODUCTION

Given its widespread use, the public and psychologists alike are concerned well-nigh the impact of violent video game play. In fact, a swell number of studies accept addressed the furnishings of exposure to violent video games (where the master goal is to harm other game characters) on aggression and assailment‐related variables. Meta‐analyses take shown that playing vehement video games is associated with increased assailment in the thespian (Anderson et al., 2010; Greitemeyer & Mügge, 2014). The present longitudinal written report examines the idea that violent video game play likewise affects the actor's social network, suggesting that business organisation nigh the harmful effects of playing tearing video games on a societal level is even more warranted.

one.i. Theoretical perspective

When explaining the effects of playing violent video games, researchers ofttimes refer to the Full general Aggression Model (GAM) proposed past Anderson & Bushman (2002). According to this theoretical model, person and situation variables (sometimes interactively) may affect a person's internal land, consisting of cognition, bear on, and arousal. This internal state then affects how events are perceived and interpreted. Based on this conclusion process, the person behaves more or less aggressively in a social encounter. For example, playing violent video games is assumed to increment aggressive cognition and affect, which in turn results in behavioral assailment. An extension of this model further assumes that increased aggression due to previous violent video game play may instigate an aggression escalation wheel in that the victim besides behaves aggressively (cf. Anderson & Bushman, 2018, Figure five). The nowadays research tested key predictions derived from the GAM and its extension, that (a) tearing video game play is associated with increased aggression in the player and that (b) individuals who are continued to the player will as well become more aggressive.

1.2. Effects of fierce video game play on aggression

The relationship between violent video game play and aggression has been examined in studies employing cross‐sectional, longitudinal, and experimental designs. Cantankerous‐sectional correlational studies typically show a positive relationship between the amount of vehement video game play and aggression in real‐world contexts (e.g., Gentile, Lynch, Linder, & Walsh, 2004; Krahé & Möller, 2004). Several longitudinal studies take been conducted, showing that habitual violent video game play predicts later aggression even afterwards controlling for initial aggressiveness (e.thousand., Anderson, Buckley, & Carnagey, 2008). That violent video game play has a causal bear on on aggression and related data processing has been demonstrated by experimental work (east.g., Anderson & Carnagey, 2009; Gabbiadini & Riva, 2018). Finally, meta‐analyses corroborated that fierce video game play significantly increases aggressive thoughts, hostile affect, and aggressive behavior (Anderson et al., 2010; Greitemeyer & Mügge, 2014). Some studies failed to find significant effects (due east.g., McCarthy, Coley, Wagner, Zengel, & Basham, 2016). However, given that the typical result of violent video games on aggression is not large, it is to be expected that not all studies reveal significant furnishings.

ane.3. The contagious furnishings of aggression

Abundant evidence has been collected that aggression and violence can exist contagious (Dishion, & Tipsord, 2011; Huesmann, 2012; Jung, Busching, & Krahé, 2019). Indeed, the best predictor of (retaliatory) aggression is arguably previous violent victimization (Anderson et al., 2008; Goldstein, Davis, & Herman, 1975). However, even the observation of violence tin can atomic number 82 to increased violence in the hereafter (Widom, 1989). Overall, it is a well‐known finding that assailment begets further assailment. Given that violent video game play increases assailment, it thus may well exist that this increased aggression then has an touch on on people with whom the actor is connected.

Correlational research provides initial testify for the idea that the level of people's aggression is indeed associated with how often their friends play violent video games (Greitemeyer, 2018). In particular, participants who did non play violent video games were more than aggressive the more than their friends played violent video games. Notwithstanding, due to the cross‐sectional design, no conclusions near the direction of the effect are possible. It may be that violent video game players influence their friends (social influence), but it is likewise believable that similar people attract each other (homophily) or that there is some shared environmental factor that influences the behavior of both the players and their friends (confounding). That is, information technology is unclear whether indeed aggression due to playing violent video games spreads or whether the effect is reversed, such that aggressive people are prone to befriend others who are attracted to fierce video game play. Moreover, it is possible that some 3rd variable afflicted both, participants' reported aggression and their friends' corporeality of violent video game play. There is also the possibility that people are unsure about the extent to which their friends play fierce video games. In this case, they may perceive their friends equally behaving aggressively then (wrongly) infer that the friends play violent video games. To disentangle these possibilities and to show that the effect of vehement video game play (i.e., increased aggression in the actor) indeed has an impact on the actor's social network, relationships amongst variables have to be assessed over time while covarying prior aggression (Bond & Bushman, 2017; Christakis & Fowler, 2013).

Verheijen, Burk, Stoltz, van den Berg, and Cillessen (2018) tested the idea that players of violent video games accept a long‐term impact on their social network. These authors establish that participants' exposure to fierce video games increased their friend'south aggressive beliefs 1 year later. Withal, given that the authors did not examine whether the violent video game player'southward increased aggression accounts for the impact on their friend's aggressive beliefs, it is unknown whether violent video game play indeed instigates an aggression bicycle. For case, players of violent video games may influence their friends so that these friends will as well play violent video games. Whatever increases in aggression could so be an consequence of the friends playing vehement video games on their own.

1.4. The present research

The present study examines the longitudinal association betwixt the participant's aggression and their friends' amount of trigger-happy video game play, employing an egocentric networking approach (Stark & Krosnick, 2017). In egoistic networking analyses, participants provide self‐reports simply also report on how they perceive their friends. In the post-obit, and in line with Greitemeyer (2018), the friends were treated as the players and the participant was treated equally their friends' social network. Delight note that ties between the participant's friends (i.e., whether friends also know each other) were non assessed (Greitemeyer, 2018; Mötteli & Dohle, 2019), because this information was not needed for testing the hypothesis that participants become more than aggressive if their friends play violent video games. Information technology was expected that friends' amount of vehement video game play at Time 1 would predict the participant'southward aggression at Fourth dimension two even when controlling for the impact of the participant's aggression and amount of trigger-happy video game play at Fourth dimension 1. Information technology was farther examined whether friends' assailment at Fourth dimension 1 would account for the touch of friends' amount of violent video game play at Fourth dimension i on the participant's assailment at Time ii. Such findings would provide suggestive bear witness that violent video game play may instigate an aggression cycle. The study received ethical approving from the Internal Review Board for Ethical Questions by the Scientific Upstanding Commission of the University of Innsbruck. The data and materials are openly accessible at https://osf.io/jp8ew/.

ii. METHOD

2.ane. Participants

Participants were citizens of the U.Due south. who took role on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Considering it was unknown how many of the participants will complete both questionnaires, no power analyses were conducted a priori just a big number of participants was run. At Time 1, there were 2,502 participants (ane,376 females, 1,126 males; mean age = 35.7 years, SD =11.viii). Of these, 980 participants (522 females, 458 males; hateful age = 38.9 years, SD =12.5) completed the questionnaire at Time 2. Fourth dimension i and Time ii were 6 months apart. There were no data exclusions, and all participants were run earlier whatever analyses were performed. The questionnaire included some further questions (e.one thousand., participant's perceived deprivation) that are not relevant for the present purpose and are reported elsewhere (Greitemeyer & Sagioglou, 2018).1 Given that the questionnaire was relatively short, no attending checks were employed.

two.2. Procedure and measures

Procedure and measures were very similar to Greitemeyer (2018), with the main difference that individuals participated at two time points (instead of one). After providing demographics, self‐reported aggressive beliefs was assessed. As in previous research (east.one thousand., Krahé & Möller, 2010), participants indicated for 10 items how often they had shown the respective behavior in the past half dozen months. Sample items are: "I take pushed another person" and "I have spread gossip nearly people I don't like" (v items each address physical aggression and relational aggression, respectively). All items were rated on a scale from ane (never) to 5 (very often), and scores were averaged. Participants were then asked nearly their amount of violent video game play, employing one item: "How oft do y'all play tearing video games (where the goal is to impairment other game characters)?" (i =never to 7 =very often).

Afterwards, participants learned that they volition be asked questions about people they feel closest to. These may exist friends, coworkers, neighbors, relatives. They should answer questions for three contacts with whom they talked about important matters in the last few months. For each friend, they reported the level of aggression (αs betwixt = 0.90 and 0.91) and the amount of violent video game play, employing the same questions as for themselves. Responses to the three friends were and then averaged. Finally, participants were thanked and asked what they idea this experiment was trying to study, merely none noted the hypothesis that their friend's amount of violent video game play would bear on their own level of aggression. At Fourth dimension 2, the same questions were employed. Reliabilities for how participants perceived the level of aggression for each friend were between 0.89 and 0.ninety.

3. RESULTS

Descriptive statistics, intercorrelations, and internal consistencies of all measures are shown in Table 1.

Table 1

Means, standard deviations, and bivariate correlations

Chiliad SD i 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
ane. Participant's corporeality of trigger-happy video game play (T1) ii.74 two.09
ii. Participant's aggression (T1) 1.38 0.52 .xv .89
3. Friends' amount of trigger-happy video game play (T1) 2.28 1.31 .59 .18 .44
4. Friends' aggression (T1) 1.39 0.49 .14 .69 .25 .76
5. Participant'due south corporeality of violent video game play (T2) 2.50 one.93 .83 .12 .55 .12
6. Participant'due south aggression (T2) ane.30 0.45 .13 .50 .xviii .43 .14 .88
vii. Friends' amount of violent video game play (T2) two.18 1.27 .55 .18 .69 .22 .61 .22 .51
viii. Friends' assailment (T2) i.33 0.44 .13 .40 .19 .51 .13 .74 .25 .79

three.ane. Time 1 (North = 2,502)

The relationship between the amount of violent video game play and reported assailment was meaning, both for the participant and the friends. That is, violent video game play was associated with increased aggression in the actor and participants perceived their friends who play more violent video games to be more than aggressive than their less‐playing friends. Participant's and friends' amount of violent video game play as well equally their level of reported aggression, respectively, were also positively associated, indicating that participants perceived their friends to exist like to them. Most importantly, participant's aggression was significantly associated with friends' amount of violent video game play.ii

It was then examined whether friends' amount of tearing video game play is nonetheless associated with the participant'southward aggression when controlling for the participant'due south amount of tearing video game play. Participant sex (coded 1 = male, 2 = female) and age were included as covariates. In fact, a bootstrapping analysis showed that the touch on of friends' corporeality of fierce video game play remained significant (point estimate = 0.08, SE = 0.02, t = 4.72, p < .001, 95% confidence interval [CI] = [0.05, 0.11]). Participant'south amount of tearing video game play (point estimate = 0.03, SE = 0.01, t = two.18, p = .029, 95% CI = [0.00, 0.05]) and the interaction were besides significant (indicate estimate = −0.01, SE = 0.00, t = 2.41, p = .016, 95% CI = [−0.02, −0.00]). At depression levels of the participant'south amount of violent video game play (− 1 SD, equals that the participant does not play trigger-happy video games in the present data fix), friends' amount of violent video game play was associated with the participant'southward aggression (point guess = 0.07, SE = 0.01, t = 5.06, p < .001, 95% CI = [0.04, 0.ten]). At high levels of the participant'south amount of violent video game play ( + i SD), friends' amount of vehement video game play was also associated with the participant'southward aggression (point guess = 0.03, SE = 0.01, t = 3.14, p = .002, 95% CI = [0.01, 0.06]), but the consequence was less pronounced. Participants were thus nigh strongly affected by whether their social network plays violent video games when they do not play violent video games themselves (Figure 1). Participant sex was not significantly associated with the participant's aggression (indicate approximate = −0.04, SE = 0.02, t = 1.95, p = .052, 95% CI = [−0.09, 0.00]), whereas age was (indicate guess = −0.01, SE = 0.00, t = 7.84, p < .001, 95% CI = [−0.009, −0.005]).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is AB-45-635-g001.jpg

Uncomplicated slopes of the interactive effect of friends' amount of violent video game play and the participant's amount of violent video game play on the participant'south aggression, controlling for participant sex and age (Fourth dimension i, Due north = ii,502)

iii.2. Time one and Time 2 (N = 980)

To examine the impact of friends' amount of fierce video game play on the participant's aggression over time, a cross‐lagged regression analysis was performed on the data. Participant'south amount of trigger-happy video game play, friends' amount of violent video game play, participant's aggression at Time 1, every bit well as participant sex and age were used every bit predictors for participant'south aggression at Time 2. The overall regression was significant, F(5,974) = 68.92, R two = 0.26, p < .001. Most importantly, friends' amount of tearing video game play at Time ane significantly predicted participant's assailment at Time 2, t = ii.60, β = .09, 95% CI = (0.02, 0.16), p = .009. Participant'southward aggression showed high stability, t = sixteen.77, β = .48, 95% CI = (0.42, 0.53), p < .001, whereas the participant's amount of violent video game play at Time i did not significantly predict the participant's aggression at Time 2, t = ane.77, β = −.07, 95% CI = (− 0.14, 0.01), p = .077 (Figure 2).3 , 4 Participant sex as well received a pregnant regression weight, t = 2.08, β = −.06, 95% CI = (−0.12, −0.00), p = .038, whereas historic period did not, t = 1.93, β = −.06, 95% CI = (−0.12, 0.00), p = .054. The contrary event that the participant'due south assailment at Fourth dimension ane predicts their friends' amount of trigger-happy video game play at Time 2 when decision-making for the participant'south amount of violent video game play and friends' amount of violent video game play at Time 1, as well equally participant sexual activity and historic period, was not significant, t = 0.67, β = .02, 95% CI = (−0.03, 0.06), p = .504.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is AB-45-635-g002.jpg

Participant's assailment at Time ii simultaneously predicted past friends' amount of violent video game play, participant'south aggression, and participant's amount of violent video game play at Time 1. Participant sex activity and age were controlled for, but were not included in the figure (come across the main text for the touch on of participant sex and historic period). *p < .01, ** p < .001 (North = 980)

Finally, information technology was examined whether the impact of friends' corporeality of tearing video game play at Fourth dimension 1 on the participant'south aggression at Fourth dimension two would exist mediated by friends' level of aggression at Fourth dimension 1 (while controlling for the participant's assailment and amount of violent video game play at Time i also as participant sexual practice and age). A bootstrapping assay (with 5.000 iterations) showed that the impact of friends' level of aggression at Fourth dimension 1 on the participant's aggression at Fourth dimension two was significant (point judge = 0.16, SE = 0.04, t = 4.28, p < .001, 95% CI = [0.09, 0.23]). Participant's assailment at Time ane was as well a pregnant predictor (point judge = 0.34, SE = 0.03, t = 10.xix, p < .001, 95% CI = [0.27, 0.40]). Friends' amount of trigger-happy video game play at Fourth dimension 1 (point estimate = 0.03, SE = 0.01, t = i.82, p = .069, 95% CI = [−0.00, 0.05]) and participant'south amount of violent video game play at Time 1 (bespeak estimate = −0.01, SE = 0.01, t = 1.65, p = .099, 95% CI = [−0.03, 0.00]) were non pregnant predictors. Participant sex significantly predicted the participant's aggression at Time 2 (point estimate = −0.06, SE = 0.03, t = ii.31, p = .021, 95% CI = [−0.11, −0.01]), whereas age did non (indicate estimate = −0.00, SE = 0.00, t = i.90, p = .058, 95% CI = [−0.00, 0.00]). The indirect issue was significantly different from zero (point guess = 0.01, 95% CI = [.00, 0.02]), suggesting that participants are more aggressive if their friends play violent video games for the reason that these friends are more aggressive. Figure 3 displays a simplified version of this mediation issue, based on regression coefficients and without controlling for the participant's aggression at Time ane, the participant's amount of violent video game play at Time one, participant sexual activity, and historic period.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is AB-45-635-g003.jpg

Mediation of the impact of friends' fierce video game exposure (VVE) at Time 1 on the participant'due south aggression at Time 2 by friends' aggression at Time i. All paths are meaning. β* = the coefficient from friends' VVE at Time i to the participant's aggression at Time 2 when controlling for friends' assailment at Fourth dimension 1 (N = 980)

four. DISCUSSION

Violent video games take an impact on the player's aggression (Anderson et al., 2010; Greitemeyer & Mügge, 2014), only—every bit the present report shows—they also increment aggression in the player's social network. In particular, participants who do non play fierce video games reported to exist more ambitious the more their friends play fierce video games. Mediation analyses showed that the increased aggression in the friends accounted for the human relationship betwixt friends' corporeality of violent video game play and the participant'south aggression. Because changes in aggression over time were assessed, the present written report provides evidence for the hypothesized consequence that tearing video game play is associated with increased aggression in the player, which then instigates aggression in their social network. Chiefly, the impact of the participant'south amount of violent video game play was controlled for, indicating that the relationship between friends' amount of tearing video game play and the participant'south aggression is not due to the friends existence similar to the participants. Moreover, the reverse effect that aggressive people volition go attracted to others who play violent video games was non reliable. The present research thus documents the directional effects that vehement video games is associated with increased aggression in the player and that this increased assailment then has an impact on people with whom the player is connected.

Overall, the present study provides comprehensive support for key hypotheses derived from the GAM and its extension (Anderson & Bushman, 2018). It shows that violent video game play is associated with increased aggression in the actor and it documents that others who are connected to players might exist as well affected even when decision-making for their ain corporeality of tearing video game play. To the best of my cognition, this study is the kickoff that shows that considering fierce video game players are more than ambitious their friends will go ambitious, too. Previous research either employed a cross‐exclusive design and thus could not address the management of the result (Greitemeyer, 2018) or did non examine whether the effect of violent video game play (i.eastward., increased assailment) indeed spreads (Verheijen et al., 2018). As proposed past the GAM and its extension (Anderson & Bushman, 2018), increased aggression in violent video game players appears to instigate an aggression escalation wheel (cf. Anderson et al., 2008).

It is noteworthy, yet, that the longitudinal effect of the participant'south amount of violent video game play at Time 1 on the participant's aggression at Fourth dimension 2 was not reliable. Hence, although there were meaning correlations between participants' assailment and their violent video game use at both fourth dimension points, the present written report does non show that repeatedly playing vehement video games leads to long‐term changes in aggression. Still, a recent meta‐analysis of the long‐term furnishings of playing violent video games confirmed that violent video game play does increase concrete aggression over fourth dimension (Prescott, Sargent, & Hull, 2018), although the upshot size was relatively small-scale (β = 0.xi) and thus unmarried studies that produce nonsignificant results are to be expected. Chiefly, in the nowadays study, a single‐detail measure out of violent video game play was employed. In contrast, previous research on the relationship between violent video game play and the actor's aggression has often employed multi‐item measurement scales that are typically more reliable and precise (for an overview, Busching et al., 2015). Hence, it may well be that due to the limitations of the unmarried‐item measure of the participant's amount of violent video game play the relationship betwixt participants' violent game play and their ambitious behavior was artificially reduced.

Even though the longitudinal design allows ruling out a host of culling explanations for the impact of tearing video games on the player's social network, causality tin can only inferred by using an experimental design. Future inquiry may thus randomly assign participants to play a violent or nonviolent video game (players) and assesses their aggression against new participants (partners). It tin be expected that the partners suffer more aggression when the role player had played a violent, compared to a irenic, video game. Afterwards, it could exist tested whether the partner of a violent video game player is more aggressive than a partner of a irenic video game actor. Given that the partner is non exposed to any video games, firm causal conclusions could be drawn that violent video game play affects assailment in people who are continued to tearing video game players. Information technology could exist also tested whether the partner of a violent video game player would not merely be more probable to retaliate against the player, merely as well against a tertiary political party. In fact, previous research into displaced aggression has shown that people may react aggressively against a target that is innocent of any wrongdoing later they take been provoked by another person (Marcus‐Newhall, Pedersen, Carlson, & Miller, 2000). It may thus well be that the effect of playing trigger-happy video games spreads in social networks and that even people who are just indirectly linked to violent video game players are afflicted.

An of import limitation of the present egocentric network data is the reliance on the participant'due south perception of their social network, leaving the possibility that participants did not accurately perceive their friends. Information technology is noteworthy that participants perceived their friends to exist highly similar to them. In this regard, it is of import to keep in mind that participants always provided self‐ratings first, followed by perceptions of their friends. It is thus conceivable that participants used their self‐ratings as anchors for the perceptions of their friends. Such a tendency, still, would reduce the unique effect of friends' corporeality of trigger-happy video game play on the participant's aggression when controlling for the participant's amount of violent video game play. The finding that participants in particular who do not play vehement video games reported to be more aggressive if their friends play violent video games also suggests that the impact of vehement video games on the histrion's social network is not due to participants providing both self‐reports and how they perceive their friends. Finally, rather than by their friends' objective qualities, people'south behavior should be more probable to be affected by their subjective perceptions of their friends.

As noted in the introduction, participants may not be aware of the extent to which their friends play violent video games and hence used the perception of how aggressive their friends are as an anchor for estimating their friends' amount of violent video game play. Importantly, notwithstanding, the participant's assailment at Fourth dimension 2 was significantly predicted by friends' amount of fierce video game play at Fourth dimension 1 even when controlling for friends' level of aggression at Fourth dimension 1 (meet Figure three). Moreover, whereas aggression might be used for estimating vehement video game exposure of the friends, participants should be well aware of the extent to which they play violent video games so that anchoring furnishings for participant'south self‐reports are unlikely. However, given that it cannot be completely ruled out that the correlation between violent game play of friends at Fourth dimension 1 and aggressive behavior of participants at Time two reflects a pseudocorrelation that is adamant by the correlation between ambitious beliefs of friends at Fourth dimension 1 and aggressive behavior of the participant at Time 2, futurity research that employs sociocentric network analyses where information about the friends is provided by the friends themselves would be informative.

Another limitation is the employment of cocky‐report measures to appraise aggressive beliefs. Self‐report measures are quite transparent, so participants may accept rated themselves more than favorably than is actually warranted. In fact, hateful scores of reported aggressive beliefs were quite low. This reduced variance, however, typically diminishes associations with other constructs. In any example, observing how bodily aggressive behavior is influenced by the social network'southward fierce video game play would be an important endeavor for future work. It also has to be acknowledged that some participants may have reported on dissimilar friends at Time one and Time 2. Future enquiry would be welcome that ensures that participants consider the same friends at different time points.

Future research may also shed some further light on the psychological processes. In the nowadays study, the violent video game players' higher levels of assailment accounted for the relationship between their amount of violent video game play and the participants' reported aggression. Information technology would exist interesting to examine why the players' assailment influences the aggression level of their social network. Ane possibility is that witnessing increased assailment past others (who play vehement video games) leads to greater credence of norms palliating aggression, which are known to be an antecedent of aggressive beliefs (Huesmann & Guerra, 1997). After all, if others behave aggressively, why should 1 refrain from engaging in the aforementioned beliefs.

Some other limitation of the present work is that information technology was not assessed how participants and their friends play violent video games. A recent survey (Lenhart, Smith, Anderson, Duggan, & Perrin, 2015) showed that many video game users play video games together with their friends, either cooperatively or competitively. This is insofar noteworthy as there might be some overlap between participants' and their friends' violent video game play. Moreover, cooperative video games have been shown to increase prosocial tendencies (Greitemeyer, 2013; Greitemeyer & Cox, 2013; but see Verheijen, Stoltz, van den Berg, & Cillessen, 2019) and subtract aggression (Velez, Greitemeyer, Whitaker, Ewoldsen, & Bushman, 2016). In contrast, competitive video game play increases aggressive affect and behavior (due east.grand., Adachi & Willoughby, 2016). Hence, hereafter enquiry should examine more than closely whether participants play violent video games on their ain, competitively, or cooperatively. The latter may show some positive effects of video game play, both on the player and the player's friends, whereas opposing furnishings should be establish for competitive video games.

To obtain high statistical power and thus to increase the probability to detect pregnant effects, information were collected via an online survey. The electric current sample was drawn from the MTurk population (for a review of the tendency to rely on MTurk samples in social and personality psychology, see Anderson et al., 2019). Samples drawn from MTurk are not demographically representative of the U.South. population as a whole. For instance, MTurk samples are disproportionally young and female and they are better educated just tend to be unemployed (for a review, Keith, Tay, & Harms, 2017). On the other hand, MTurk samples are more representative of the U.South. population than are college student samples (Paolacci & Chandler, 2014) and the pool of participants is geographically diverse. Moreover, MTurk participants appear to be more than attentive to survey instructions than are undergraduate students (Hauser & Schwarz, 2016). However, future research on the touch on of vehement video game play on the actor'south social network that employs other samples would improve the generalizability of the present findings.

In conclusion, violent video game play is non merely associated with increased aggression in the player merely as well in the player's social network. In fact, increased aggression due to violent video game play appears to instigate further aggression in the player'south social network. This report thus provides suggestive prove that non only players of violent video games are more aggressive, but also individuals go more than aggressive who exercise not play trigger-happy video games themselves but are continued to others who do play.

Notes

Greitemeyer T. The contagious impact of playing violent video games on aggression: Longitudinal evidence. Aggressive Beliefs. 2019;45:635–642. 10.1002/ab.21857 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

ENDNOTES

aneParticipant'southward perceived deprivation was positively related to both violent video game exposure, r(2,502) = 0.08, p < .001, and reported aggression, r(two,502) = 0.14, p < .001. Withal, the relationship between tearing video game exposure and reported aggression, r(2,502) = 0.15, p < .001, was relatively unaffected when controlling for perceived deprivation, r(2,499) = 0.14, p < .001.

2Given that the measures of violent video game exposure and aggressive behavior violated the normal distribution, Spearman's ρ coefficients were besides calculated. Nonetheless, the pattern of finding was very similar (e.yard., the crucial relationship betwixt the participant's assailment and friends' corporeality of violent video game play was 0.18 [Pearson] and 0.17 [Spearman]). All these analyses can be obtained from the author upon request.

3When dropping friends' corporeality of violent video game play from the analysis, the participant's amount of violent video game play at Fourth dimension 1 still did not predict participant's aggression at Fourth dimension 2, t = 0.44, β = −.01, 95% CI = (− 0.02, 0.01), p = .657 (when controlling for participant'south aggression at Fourth dimension ane, participant sexual practice, and age).

4Given that violent video games primarily model physical aggression, tearing video games should have a stronger effect on the actor's concrete aggression than on other types of assailment. In fact, the affect of the participant's amount of violent video game play at Time one on the participant's physical aggression at Time 2, t = 1.49, β = .04, 95% CI = (− 0.00, 0.02), p = .136 (when decision-making for the participant's physical aggression at Time ane), was more pronounced than the touch on on the participant'southward relational aggression at Time ii, t = 0.52, β = .02, 95% CI = (− 0.01, 0.02), p = .603 (when controlling for the participant'south relational assailment at Time 1), but both effects were non pregnant.

References

REFERENCES

  • Adachi, P. J. C. , & Willoughby, T. (2016). The longitudinal association betwixt competitive video game play and assailment amid adolescents and immature adults. Child Evolution, 87, 1877–1892. 10.1111/cdev.12556 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Anderson, C. A. , Allen, J. J. , Plante, C. , Quigley‐McBride, A. , Lovett, A. , & Rokkum, J. N. (2019). The MTurkification of social and personality psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45, 842–850. x.1177%2F0146167218798821 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Anderson, C. A. , Buckley, G. E. , & Carnagey, N. L. (2008). Creating your own hostile surround: A laboratory examination of trait aggressiveness and the violence escalation cycle. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 462–473. 10.1177/0146167207311282 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Anderson, C. A. , & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Homo aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27–51. 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Anderson, C. A. , & Bushman, B. J. (2018). Media violence and the General Aggression Model. Journal of Social Issues, 74, 386–413. x.1111/josi.12275 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Anderson, C. A. , & Carnagey, Northward. L. (2009). Causal effects of fierce sports video games on aggression: Is information technology competitiveness or violent content? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 731–739. ten.1016/j.jesp.2009.04.019 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Anderson, C. A. , Sakamoto, A. , Gentile, D. A. , Ihori, N. , Shibuya, A. , Yukawa, S. , … Kobayashi, 1000. (2008). Longitudinal effects of tearing video games on aggression in Japan and the United states. Pediatrics, 122, e1067–e1072. 10.1542/peds.2008-1425 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Anderson, C. A. , Shibuya, A. , Ihori, Due north. , Swing, Eastward. L. , Bushman, B. J. , Sakamoto, A. , … Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game furnishings on assailment, empathy, and prosocial behavior in Eastern and Western countries. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 151–173. 10.1037/a0018251 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bond, R. Chiliad. , & Bushman, B. J. (2017). The contagious spread of violence amid United states of america adolescents through social networks. American Periodical of Public Health, 107, 288–294. 10.2105/AJPH.2016.303550 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Busching, R. , Gentile, D. A. , Krahé, B. , Möller, I. , Khoo, A. , Walsh, D. A. , & Anderson, C. A. (2015). Testing the reliability and validity of different measures of tearing video game use in the United States, Singapore, and Frg. Psychology of Popular Media Civilisation, four, 97–111. [Google Scholar]
  • Christakis, Due north. A. , & Fowler, J. H. (2013). Social contagion theory: Examining dynamic social networks and human behavior. Statistics in Medicine, 32, 556–577. 10.1002/sim.5408 [PMC complimentary article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Dishion, T. J. , & Tipsord, J. Chiliad. (2011). Peer contagion in child and adolescent social and emotional development. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 189–214. 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100412 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gabbiadini, A. , & Riva, P. (2018). The alone gamer: Social exclusion predicts fierce video game preferences and fuels aggressive inclinations in adolescent players. Aggressive Behavior, 44, 113–124. 10.1002/ab.21735 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gentile, D. A. , Lynch, P. J. , Linder, J. R. , & Walsh, D. A. (2004). The effects of trigger-happy video game habits on boyish hostility, ambitious behaviors, and school functioning. Journal of Boyhood, 27, five–22. 10.1016/j.boyhood.2003.10.002 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Goldstein, J. H. , Davis, R. Westward. , & Herman, D. (1975). Escalation of aggression: Experimental studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 162–170. 10.1037/h0076241 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Greitemeyer, T. (2013). Playing video games cooperatively increases empathic concern. Social Psychology, 44, 408–413. ten.1027/1864-9335/a000154 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Greitemeyer, T. (2018). The spreading impact of playing violent video games on assailment. Computers in Human Behavior, fourscore, 216–219. x.1016/j.chb.2017.xi.022 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Greitemeyer, T. , & Cox, C. (2013). There's no "I" in team: Effects of cooperative video games on cooperative behavior: Video games and cooperation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 224–228. 10.1002/ejsp.1940 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Greitemeyer, T. , & Mügge, D. O. (2014). Video games do affect social outcomes: A meta‐analytic review of the effects of violent and prosocial video game play. Personality and Social Psychology Message, twoscore, 578–589. 10.1177/0146167213520459 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Greitemeyer, T. , & Sagioglou, C. (2018). The impact of personal relative deprivation on aggression over time. The Journal of Social Psychology, 3–7. 10.1080/00224545.2018.1549013 [PMC costless article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hauser, D. J. , & Schwarz, North. (2016). Attentive Turkers: MTurk participants perform better on online attention checks than do bailiwick pool participants. Behavior Research Methods, 48, 400–407. 10.3758/s13428-015-0578-z [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Huesmann, L. R. (2012). The contagion of violence: The extent, the processes, and the outcomes. Social and economic costs of violence: Workshop summary (pp. 63–69). Washington, DC: IOM (Institute of Medicine) and NRC (National, Research Quango). [Google Scholar]
  • Huesmann, L. R. , & Guerra, Northward. G. (1997). Children'south normative beliefs about aggression and aggressive beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 408–419. ten.1037/0022-3514.72.two.408 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Jung, J. , Busching, R. , & Krahé, B. (2019). Communicable aggression from one's peers: A longitudinal and multilevel analysis. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 13, e12433 ten.1111/spc3.12433 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Keith, M. Yard. , Tay, L. , & Harms, P. D. (2017). Systems perspective of Amazon Mechanical Turk for organizational research: Review and recommendations. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1359 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01359 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Krahé, B. , & Möller, I. (2004). Playing trigger-happy electronic games, hostile attributional style, and aggression‐related norms in High german adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 53–69. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.ten.006 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Krahé, B. , & Möller, I. (2010). Longitudinal effects of media violence on assailment and empathy among German adolescents. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31, 401–409. 10.1016/j.appdev.2010.07.003 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lenhart, A. , Smith, A. , Anderson, M. , Duggan, M. , & Perrin, A. (2015). Teens, technology and friendships Pew Cyberspace and American Life Project. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/08/06/teens-technology-and-friendships/
  • Marcus‐Newhall, A. , Pedersen, W. C. , Carlson, M. , & Miller, N. (2000). Displaced aggression is alive and well: A meta‐analytic review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 670–689. 10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.670 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • McCarthy, R. J. , Coley, South. L. , Wagner, M. F. , Zengel, B. , & Basham, A. (2016). Does playing video games with tearing content temporarily increase aggressive inclinations? A pre‐registered experimental study. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 13–19. 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.10.009 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Mötteli, Southward. , & Dohle, South. (2019). Egocentric social network correlates of concrete activity. Journal of Sport and Health Science, ii–eight. 10.1016/j.jshs.2017.01.002 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Paolacci, Thou. , & Chandler, J. (2014). Within the Turk: Understanding Mechanical Turk as a participant pool. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 184–188. ten.1177/0963721414531598 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Prescott, A. T. , Sargent, J. D. , & Hull, J. Thou. (2018). Metaanalysis of the relationship between trigger-happy video game play and physical aggression over time. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115, 9882–9888. 10.1073/pnas.1611617114 [PMC complimentary commodity] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Stark, T. H. , & Krosnick, J. A. (2017). GENSI: A new graphical tool to collect ego‐centered network data. Social Networks, 48, 36–45. 10.1016/j.socnet.2016.07.007 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Velez, J. A. , Greitemeyer, T. , Whitaker, J. L. , Ewoldsen, D. R. , & Bushman, B. J. (2016). Violent video games and reciprocity: The attenuating effects of cooperative game play on subsequent aggression. Communication Research, 43, 447–467. 10.1177/0093650214552519 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Verheijen, One thousand. P. , Burk, W. J. , Stoltz, S. E. 1000. J. , van den Berg, Y. H. M. , & Cillessen, A. H. North. (2018). Friendly burn: Longitudinal effects of exposure to trigger-happy video games on aggressive behavior in adolescent friendship dyads. Aggressive Behavior, 44, 257–267. 10.1002/ab.21748 [PMC costless article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Verheijen, G. P. , Stoltz, Southward. Due east. 1000. J. , van den Berg, Y. H. M. , & Cillessen, A. H. North. (2019). The influence of competitive and cooperative video games on beliefs during play and friendship quality in adolescence. Computers in Human being Beliefs, 91, 297–304. 10.1016/j.chb.2018.10.023 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Widom, C. S. (1989). Does violence beget violence? A disquisitional examination of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 3–28. ten.1037/0033-2909.115.2.287 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

hicksonwastand.blogspot.com

Source: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6790614/

0 Response to "Apa Review Confirms Link Between Playing Violent Video Games and Aggression"

Post a Comment

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel